How Do Frequency and Duration of Messaging Affect Impression
Development in Computer-Mediated Communication?12
Yuliang Liu
(Southern Illinois University Edwardsville, United States
yliu@siue.edu
Dean Ginther
(Texas A&M University-Commerce, United States
Dean_Ginther@tamu-commerce.edu)
Paul Zelhart
(Texas A&M University-Commerce, United States
Paul_Zelhart@tamu-commerce.edu)
Abstract: Computer-mediated Communication (CMC) has been commonly
compared to face-to-face (FtF) communication in recent CMC literature.
Research comparisons suggested depersonalizing effects of CMC. However,
this experimental study indicates that CMC is a potentially viable mode
of social-emotion-oriented communication. In this study, the effects of
frequency and duration of messaging on impression development in CMC were
investigated. Undergraduate participants were randomly assigned to each
of the four experimental groups. For a period of two weeks, participants
monitored discussion lists that differed in relation to the frequency and
duration of messaging in asynchronous CMC environments. ANOVA results indicated
that duration and frequency had significant main effects on impression
development in asynchronous CMC environments. No interaction effects were
found. The results of this study not only theoretically support the social-emotion-oriented
model in CMC, but also lay foundations for further research in many popular
types of interactive CMC environments, including e-learning, e-commerce,
and e-health.
Keywords: frequency of messaging, duration of messaging, impression
development, nonverbal cues, computer-mediated communication, social-emotion-oriented
model, social information processing model
Categories: C.2, J.4, K.3, K.4, K.6
[1] This
study was a portion of the first author's doctoral dissertation at Texas
A&M University-Commerce in 2000. For detail information, please refer
to Liu, Y.: "The effects of nonverbal cues on impression formation
in computer-mediated communication: An exploratory study"; Dissertation
Abstracts International, 61, 4 (2000), AAT 9965844.
[2] An earlier draft of this article was
presented at the International Communication Association Annual Conference
in Washington, DC, USA in May 2001.
1 Introduction
Impression formation is a significant aspect of human interpersonal
communication and is one of the early and major topics within social psychology.
Impression formation refers to the interpersonal process by which people
employ all available information and make general judgments of other's
personality characteristics. The idea of impression formation is usually
associated with the work of Asch [Asch 1946]. Since
Asch's original work, there has been additional research about impression
formation in face-to-face (FtF) environments. Asch emphasized forming a
unified impression of the entire person through his/her trait(s) in interpersonal
communication. Most early impression formation research followed the assimilatory
tradition [O'Keefe and Delia 1982]. This tradition
investigated the determinants of differential accuracy in perception. This
type of research focused on judgments of personality traits and used a
research paradigm based upon the simple matching of perceivers and target
responses to personality inventories and questionnaires. Since the 1980's,
social cognition has contributed much to the study of impression formation.
This is a major focus shift in impression research from the problem of
accuracy to the problem of how inferences are formulated and constructed
in forming overall evaluations and impressions of others [Hampson
1990; O'Keefe and Delia 1982].
Recent FtF research [Krauss and Fussell 1996; Kraut
1978] has consistently indicated that both nonverbal cues and verbal
cues jointly affect the process of impression development. Since Asch's
[Asch 1946] classic study, the nature of impression
formation, and especially the influences of the nonverbal cues on impression
formation, has been studied extensively. According to Patterson [Patterson
1994], nonverbal cues can be managed for particular interpersonal goals,
such as engaging the other people. The nonverbal cues primarily explored
in the previous literature focused mainly on three types of cues: visible
(e. g., facial expressions, eye contact, touch, etc.), paralinguistic (e.
g., frequency, speech duration, vocal intensity, speech rate, pause, response
latency, etc.), and psychological (e. g., attention, attribution, mood,
primacy effect, and recency effect). However, the influences of language
variables on impression formation have not received much attention until
recently [Bradac and Street 1989/90]. The verbal
cues primarily explored in recent literature focused mainly on language
norms, language intensity, verbal immediacy, lexical diversity, powerful
and powerless styles, gender-related language, verbal influence strategies,
and ironic remarks.
In recent years, computer-mediated communication (CMC) has emerged as
a new communication format. CMC offers many modes of communication, both
synchronous and asynchronous. The former includes computer conferencing
and electronic databases, while the latter is made up of e-mail and bulletin
boards [Rice 1990]. Since CMC is quite different
in nature from our traditional FtF communication, it has been described
as an "altered state of communication," including altered physical
environments, altered time and space, and altered structures in communication
[Vallee, Johansen, and Sprangler 1975]. Thus, CMC
may "change the psychology and sociology of the communication process
itself" [Turoff 1978, p. 10].
Therefore, there is an urgent need to study the psychological and sociological
aspects of CMC.
Since CMC appeared several decades ago, there has accumulated much research
in recent CMC literature. Recent research indicates that there have been
two dominant research models in CMC: the task-oriented model and the social-emotion-oriented
model [Liu and Ginther 1999a]. Within the task-oriented
model, there are three variations: Social Presence Theory [Short,
Williams, and Christie 1976], Media/Information Richness Theory [Daft
and Lengel 1984, 1986], and Social Context Cues
Theory [Kiesler, Siegel, and McGuire 1984]. Since
these three variations share similarities, they are all categorized within
the "cues-filtered-out" perspective [Culnan
and Markus 1987]. According to the "cues-filtered-out" perspective,
CMC users cannot see each other and the CMC environment is restricted in
terms of nonverbal cues; therefore, CMC tends to be tasked-oriented, depersonalized,
then preventing the development of interpersonal relationships between
CMC users. Most prior CMC research tended to be consistent with the model
of the task-oriented communication and seldom dealt with CMC's emotional
content because of the lack of nonverbal cues within the CMC context [Connolly,
Jessup, and Valacich 1990; Hiltz, Johnson, and Turoff
1986].
However, contrary to the task-oriented model, Walther [Walther
1992] proposed the Social Information Processing Model to explain how
interpersonal relationships could be established in CMC environments. Specifically,
this model explains how CMC communicators process social information using
various media in CMC and FtF environments, as well as the effects of such
information on interpersonal communication. In addition, a few recent studies
have investigated how communicators are involved in social-emotion-oriented
communication [Breazeale 1999; Halbert
1999; Jacobson 1999; Lawrence
and Mongeau 1996; Lea and Spears 1991; Parks
and Floyd 1996; Rice and Love 1987; Utz
2000; Walther 1995, 1996;
Walther, Anderson, and Park 1994; Walther
and Burgoon 1992; Wright 1999]. Other studies
have found that CMC communicators are involved in both task-oriented communication
and social-emotion-oriented communication [Tangmanee
1999]. Even so, compared with research within task-oriented model,
there have not been many studies related to social-emotion-oriented model
in CMC [Liu and Ginther 1999b]. A few recent studies
have identified the existence of certain nonverbal cues in CMC and have
investigated the effects of certain nonverbal cues on social emotion development,
such as temporal aspects [Hesse, Werner, and Altman
1988; Walther and Tidwell 1995], primacy and
recency effects [Rintel and Pittam 1997], pictographs
or typographic marks and emoticons [Asteroff 1987;
Reid 1995; Thompsen and Foulger
1996]. However, there is no published literature regarding the effects
of frequency and duration on social and emotional development in CMC.
Similar to FtF environments, impression development is an important
topic in CMC [Walther 1993a, 1993b].
But there have not been many published studies in this area in recent literature.
In addition, more recent research has indicated that CMC users can adapt
both verbal and paralinguistic behaviors to communicate in CMC environments
[Reid 1995; Walther 1996; Walther,
Anderson, and Park 1994]. Walther and Burgoon [Walther
and Burgoon 1992] found that CMC groups could adapt their verbally
transmitted or textual messages to some degree of social-emotional content
in CMC environments and improve to higher levels in some relational aspects;
these subsequent levels were very close to those of FtF groups.
Based on his Social Information Processing Model and the characteristics
of CMC, Walther [Walther 1993a]constructed a quantitative
measure of impression development to be used in CMC research. The measure
predicted that impressions were expected to develop differentially over
time. Therefore, the measure was time-sensitive rather than the traditional
trait-reflective. According to Walther [Walther 1993a],
the measure can capture impressions from natural CMC interaction rather
than just checking accurate recall of manipulated traits. Meantime, Walther
[Walther 1993b] used the above measure to examine
the effects of time on the development of interpersonal impressions in
CMC vs. FtF. He found that CMC communicators developed impressions of others
gradually over five weeks, exhibiting a linear increase in impression development
and reaching the level of FtF groups. Therefore, Walther [Walther
1993b] concluded that accumulation of interpersonal impressions in
CMC over time could be conveyed by the adaptation of written language.
This is also supported by MacKinnon [MacKinnon 1995].
In MacKinnon's view of the CMC environment, one's social currency is primarily
based on the information he/she manages and the wit he/she contributes
to it rather than media richness.
In addition, Adkins and Brashers [Adkins and Brashers
1995] studied the influences of powerful and powerless language styles
on impression formation in decision-making in CMC environments. In their
experiment, powerless language style is characterized by the participant's
consistent use of hedges, hesitations, intensifiers, and tag questions.
Their experiment involved three conditions. Each condition involved two
confederates with training in preparation for the experiment. First, both
confederates used powerful language. Second, both used powerless language.
Third, one used powerful language and the other used powerless language.
Their results have indicated that a communicator using a powerful language
style in CMC environments is perceived as more attractive, credible, and
persuasive than the communicator using a powerless language style. These
perceptions may have effects on subsequent behaviors in CMC environments.
In addition, contrasting language styles made perceptions more extreme
than if a similar language style was shared by the communicators. Impression
formation could be achieved relatively quickly, within a 20-minute group
interaction, among communicators with zero-history in CMC. Therefore, Adkins
and Brashers concluded that powerful and powerless language styles had
a great influence on impression formation in CMC environments.
More recent research has indicated that CMC not only has verbal cues,
but also has nonverbal cues available that can be manipulated to develop
interpersonal relationships among CMC users [Liu and
Ginther 1999b; Walther 1992]. In CMC, many nonverbal
cues exist which include individual differences, chronemics (time of sending
and receiving a message), frequency and duration of messaging, primacy
and recency effects, gender composition, group size, or paralinguistic
cues.
The first nonverbal cue identified and studied in CMC was chronemics
or temporal aspects of CMC. Hesse, Werner, and Altman [Hesse,
Werner and Altman 1988] proposed a transactional framework to study
temporal aspects in CMC interaction. According to Hesse et al., temporal
aspects of CMC involves four major aspects: temporal scale, sequencing,
pace, and salience. In addition, according to Walther and Tidwell [Walther
and Tidwell 1995], chronemics is a very important nonverbal cue and
can be transferred via CMC.
Variations in chronemic cues can affect a communicator's judgments about
their intimacy/liking or dominance/ submissiveness in CMC relational communication.
Specifically, (a) a nighttime emotion-oriented message conveys more intimacy
than a daytime emotion-oriented one, while a nighttime task-oriented request
connotes less intimacy than a daytime task-oriented one; (b) an emotion-oriented
message sent at night indicates more equality or less dominance than an
emotion-oriented one sent in the day, while a task-oriented message sent
at night is more dominant than a task-oriented message sent in the day;
(c) a slow reply to an emotion-oriented message conveys greater intimacy/affection
than a fast one, while a slow reply to a task-oriented message connotes
less intimacy/affection than a fast one, and (d) a fast reply indicates
less dominance while a slow reply conveys greater dominance.
Other major nonverbal cues in CMC that have been identified and studied
include primacy and recency effects. According to Rintel and Pittam [Rintel
and Pittam 1997], the lack of nonverbal behavior is not an insurmountable
problem for communicators. In order to create a positive impression on
the desired receivers, there are critical factors for initial impression
formation in the opening stage in an Internet Relay Chat (IRC) environment.
These include the choice of names such as nicknames, the use of orthographic
exaggeration, extension, expansion, and paralinguistic marks such as smileys.
Therefore, according to Rintel and Pittam, the opening and closing phases
of IRC interactions are crucial for the initiation, development, and maintenance
of interpersonal relationships. Moreover, in terms of the general functions
of the strategies used, interaction management in synchronous CMC interactions
is similar to that in casual group FtF interactions. The content, structure,
and ordering of the strategies, however, are subject to modification. Therefore,
it can be inferred that interaction management in FtF may be applicable
to synchronous CMC interaction. In the opening stage, a communicator may
attempt to achieve a positive primacy impression, while attempting to achieve
a positive recency impression in the closing stage.
In addition, other researchers, such as Asteroff [Asteroff
1987], Reid [Reid 1995], and Thompsen and Foulger
[Thompsen and Foulger 1996], have consistently found
the benefits of the use of pictographs or typographic marks and emoticons
in CMC interaction because these marks can convey social emotions and reduce
perceptions of flaming. In some cases, they may convey facetiousness; in
other cases, they may convey sarcasm.
Paralinguistic cues not only include emoticons or smileys, but also
include frequency and duration of messaging, as well as latency of response.
Many previous studies have indicated that paralinguistic cues in CMC can
help written communication to be forceful and explicit [Krout,
Lewis, and Swezey 1982]. According to Lea and Spears [Lea
and Spears 1991], paralanguage is not only available in FtF interaction,
but also available in written communication in the form of typographical
marks and other characteristics of the text. Paralanguage does convey socially
shared meanings although it has no lexical meaning. Therefore, paralinguistic
cues not only facilitate the understanding of the transmitted message,
but also help define the message style from which receivers may infer certain
impressions about the communicator's personality traits.
For instance,
the appearance of typing errors in a message may imply that the sender
is in a hurry when composing the message. However, the repetitive appearance
of typing errors in a series of messages may imply that the sender is careless
and incompetent. Similarly, repetitive use of typographical marks may imply that the
sender is a lively and spontaneous person. In addition, Lea and Spears
found that spontaneously generated paralinguistic marks were related to
impression formation for both novice and experienced CMC communicators
and that whether their interpretation was positive or not completely depended
on the pre-established groups or individualistic context of the interaction.
In addition, according to Jacobson [Jacobson 1999],
CMC users do develop images of one another although researchers have proposed
that it is difficult to form impressions in computer-mediated communication.
Jacobson studied how people in virtual CMC communities envisioned each
other's offline characteristics, such as their looks and mannerisms in
"real life", based on information available online. Jacobson's
study has indicated that these impressions are based on all cues available,
as well as based on the conceptual categories and cognitive models people
use in interpreting those cues. The different prototypical effects such
as stereotypes, contribute to discrepancies between online images and offline
realities. In addition, different people use different models and categories;
this contributes to differences between online expectations and offline
experiences.
In FtF environments, frequency and duration of speech have both been
considered as good predictors of an individual's participation and impression
development in group communication. Persons with a high frequency of verbal
responses are perceived as being competent and having greater participation,
while persons with a low frequency of verbal responses are perceived as
incompetent and having less participation [Willard and
Strodtbeck 1972]. Similarly, persons with shorter duration verbal responses
are perceived as incompetent and lacking confidence, while persons with
longer duration verbal responses are perceived as competent and confident
[Koomen and Sagel 1977].
Are the effects of frequency and duration of messaging in CMC the same
as those in FtF environments? According to Rice [Rice
1984a, 1984b] and Rice and Love [Rice
and Love 1987], frequency and duration of messaging are two major aspects
related to the amount of CMC information communication. Frequency is similar
to "latency of verbal response" [Willard and
Strodtbeck 1972] and refers to how quickly a communicator responds
to begin a conversational turn. Duration is similar to the psychological
trait of "duration of verbal response" [Koomen
and Sagel 1977] and refers to how long one communicates between conversational
turns. It addition, these psychological traits are quite stable and reliable
and highly correlate with the degree of one's participation in group communication
[Hiltz and Turoff 1978]. According to Rice [Rice
1984a, 1984b] and Rice and Love [Rice
and Love 1987], sociability implies that social-emotion-oriented persons
should exhibit shorter latency of verbal responses and greater duration
of verbal responses. In addition, such persons are more likely to achieve
leadership in group interaction.
This laboratory study is intended to supplement and extend the research
by: (1) offering theoretical support for the social-emotion-oriented nature
of CMC, and (2) laying foundations for further research in many popular
types of interactive CMC environments, including e-commerce, e-health,
and e-learning.
Specifically, this study is designed to explore how the
two nonverbal cues - frequency and duration of messaging - affect impression
development in asynchronous e-mail communication.
Although CMC researchers have predicted that frequency and duration
of messaging in CMC are two important aspects of CMC information communication
[Rice and Love 1987] and that these two variables
are highly correlated with the degree of one's participation in group communication
[Hiltz and Turoff 1978], there has been little published
literature regarding the effects of frequency and duration of messaging
on impression development in CMC [Liu and Ginther
1999a, 1999b; Parks and
Floyd 1996; Walther 1996]. Thus, based on the
above statements made by Rice and Love, as well as by Hiltz and Turoff,
at least three hypotheses can be derived regarding the impression development
in CMC:
Hypothesis 1: High frequency and long duration of messaging will result
in higher mean scores, while low frequency and short duration will result
in lower mean scores, on an impression scale in CMC.
Hypothesis 2: High frequency of messaging will result in higher mean
scores, while low frequency will result in lower mean scores, on an impression
scale in CMC.
Hypothesis 3: Long durations of messaging will result in higher mean
scores, while short durations will result in lower mean scores, on an impression
scale in CMC.
In addition, according to Walther's (1992) Social Information Processing
Model described previously, CMC users can form and develop impressions
over time with their partners through both verbal and nonverbal cues in
CMC. Thus, a fourth hypothesis can be derived from Walther's model.
Hypothesis 4: The mean scores for impression development at Time 2 will
be higher than those at Time 1 on an impression scale in CMC.
2 Method
2.1 Participants
One hundred and sixteen undergraduate volunteers (Male = 33 and Female
= 83) were initially recruited from 11 psychology summer courses at a State
University in the United States in 1999. Immediately after the experimenter's
(the first author's) classroom presentation, the students were shown a
pre-recorded 5-minute video about experimental instructions, then volunteers
were asked to complete consent forms and demographic surveys. Participants
were equally divided among the four groups and were randomly assigned to
each of the four experimental groups. Eighty-three (Male = 24 and Female
= 59) participants were used for final statistical analyses, with two groups
each having a total of 20, one group having 21, and one group having 22.3
In addition, in the final analysis, each group had an almost equivalent
ratio of males and females (each group involved 6 males).
[3] Thirty-three
of one hundred and sixteen participants were excluded from final analysis
because of the participants' violation of the experimental instructions.
Specifically, 30 participants violated (b) and 3 violated (c) in Experimental
Instruction section in this paper.
2.2 Definitions of Independent Variables
In order to clearly define the independent variables, two separate pilot
studies were conducted in several Internet discussion lists to determine
typical frequency and duration of messaging. Frequency of messaging was
based on the number of messages sent to each discussion list per week and
was divided into high frequency and low frequency. According to the results
of the first pilot study, high frequency was defined as a minimum of five
messages per week and low frequency was defined as two or fewer messages
per week. Messages were sent to subjects only during the weekdays, not
on Saturday or Sunday. In the high frequency condition, each message was
separated by about 24 hours; in the low frequency condition, each message
was separated by about 48 hours. In addition, duration of messaging was
based on the length of a message sent to the discussion list by each of
the four discussants. Duration of messaging was divided into long messages
and short messages. According to the results of the second pilot study
and the APA criteria (1994) for a long quotation, a long message was defined
to include a minimum of 40 words per message and a short message was defined
to include a maximum of 20 words.
2.3 Design
This quasi-experimental factorial design involved two independent variables:
frequency and duration of messaging. Each of these independent variables
had two treatment levels. This resulted in a 2 (high frequency versus low
frequency) x 2 (long duration versus short duration) factorial design.
There were 4 treatment combinations. These four combinations were respectively
described as: condition 1 (long duration and high frequency), condition
2 (short duration and low frequency), condition 3 (long duration and low
frequency), and condition 4 (short duration and high frequency). In addition,
according to the Social Information Processing Model in CMC [Walther
1992], interpersonal relationships develop over time. Therefore, a
time factor was used as a repeated variable in this design, with the dependent
variable being measured on two occasions during the study.
2.4 Discussion Lists
The experimenter enrolled each of the subjects in one of the four discussion
list groups. Each discussion list group was moderated by the experimenter,
which meant that all electronic submissions from participants to each group
were under the control of the experimenter. In order to control the influence
of the message content, each member of each group was a "lurker"
since the subjects monitored but were not allowed to directly participate
in the discussion on the lists. The fundamental role of each subject was
to read and review the discussion list messages.
2.5 Instruments
The dependent variable in this study is the impression score of Walther's
[Walther 1993a] Impression Development Scale4.
This scale was selected for four reasons: (a) it met the time-sensitive
characteristic of CMC, (b) it was the only impression formation scale available
in CMC, (c) it has been used in CMC research in recent years, and (d) impression
formation scales in FtF are not applicable to CMC. In addition, the results
of Walther's [Walther 1993a] studies indicated that
this scale had very good construct validity. This scale has fourteen 5-interval
items. According to Walther, these 14 items (adjectives) were selectively
chosen from previous impression scales because they reflect the impression
development over time, rather than reflecting the simple physical attributes.
The 5 intervals represent strongly agree (1), agree somewhat
(2), disagree somewhat (3), strongly disagree (4),
and don't know (DK). If a 1, 2, 3, or 4 is selected as a response,
each of these responses is worth one point. Therefore, there is no difference
whether the impression item is a positive (e. g., sociable) or a negative
(e. g., unintelligent) item. A selection of "DK" is worth no
points. Thus, the maximum total score in this scale is 14 and this score
is counted to measure the degree of impression development. In other words,
the higher the total score, the more developed impression, and vice versa.
2.6 Experimental Instructions
Participants received experimental training which described their role
as both a member of their respective discussion list and a subject for
this study. In addition to a copy of the written experimental instructions,
each subject saw a 5-minute pre-recorded video describing experimental
instructions. Specifically, subjects were told that they would be required
(a) to read all messages generated on their discussion list at least once
a day, Monday through Friday; (b) to reply immediately to the list with
key ideas about each message, with the original message included in the
reply. These key ideas could be several words and were intended to verify
whether each participant had read each message on the list. If a participant
did not send key ideas within 24 hours of receiving a message on the discussion
list, he/she was reminded via email to read the unread messages on the
list. If a subject had to be reminded more than two times, he/she was removed
from further participation in the study; (c) not to discuss the experimental
task with any participants or non-participants. In order to check whether
participants did interact outside of the discussion lists, a simple 2-item
survey was administered at the end of the experiment via e-mail. If any
participant discussed the experiment task with anyone during the experiment,
he/she was eliminated from the final analysis; (d) to complete the instrument
on Saturdays during the first and the second experimental weeks; and (e)
to complete a 2-item survey on Sunday during the second week.
[4] The authors extend
their gratitude to Dr. Joseph Walther's kind permission to use his Impression
Development Scale in this study.
2.7 Experimental Stimulus
Initially, in order to control the content validity of the discussion
topic in this study, the experimenter planned to involve the participants
in a neutral discussion topic, such as statistics. But it was soon predicted
that this type of topic would hardly stimulate participants' interest.
Then, the experimenter chose another potentially interesting topic for
participants. This topic was the Littleton, Colorado school shootings that
occurred on April 20, 1999. Each of the four discussion lists discussed
this same topic. Each discussion list had four members that, for purposes
of this study, were referred to as discussants. Essentially, the subjects
in this study were told that they were "lurking" on a discussion
list with four active members.
An experimental assistant helped the experimenter to select and adapt
the messages that were later provided to each subject in their discussion
list group. The experimenter and the assistant selected a minimum of 40
archived actual messages about the Colorado school shooting from an existing
discussion list. These messages were messages that were posted to the CNN
web site at http://community.cnn.com/
on April 21, 1999. The detailed pre-scripting procedure is described as
follows.
- The experimenter and the assistant identified four members of the discussion
list in CNN web site at the http://community.cnn.com/ to serve as model
discussants. Each of the four selected list members was chosen because
they tended to produce long duration messages of high frequency. Ten messages
produced by each of the four list members were selected for later presentation
to each of the four subjects in the long duration/high frequency condition
of this study.
- The 40 messages selected to represent the long duration/high frequency
message condition were then reviewed and modified to produce the messages
for the other three experimental conditions. For the short duration/high
frequency messaging condition, the 40 messages of the long duration/high
frequency condition were reduced in length for the short duration/high
frequency condition. This reduction in length was done in such a manner
as to maintain the syntax, grammar, vocabulary, and essential meaning of
the original messages.
- The 16 messages for the long duration/low frequency condition were
selected from the 40 messages selected for the long duration/high frequency
condition. These messages were selected so that the thread of the discussion
was maintained. Similarly, the 16 messages for the short duration/low frequency
condition were selected from the 40 selected for the short duration/high
frequency condition.
In addition, in order to control the influence of the verbal cues in
list messages during the experiment, the contents of all list messages
were pre-scripted and were embedded in an ongoing dialogue, and were designed
to be as neutral as possible. Specifically, all list messages were selected
or modified to contain (a) no typing or spelling errors, (b) no "flaming"
language and personal attacks, and (c) no obscene language.
During the treatment phase of this study, the experimenter sent the
selected messages to each respective experimental discussion list with
the subject header in each message as Littleton, School Shootings. For
conditions 1 and 4, each message was sent on Monday through Friday mornings,
for conditions 2 and 3, each message was sent in the mornings on Monday
and Wednesday. For each subject-discussant, a separate e-mail account was
established, so the messages appeared to the subjects to be generated by
four separate individuals. Verification comments made by each subject were
sent to the list but intercepted by the list moderator (the experimenter)
and not distributed to list members.
2.8 Administration of the Dependent Measure
The Impression Development Scale was administered to participants on
two occasions via e-mail: once on Friday afternoon of the first experimental
week and once on Friday afternoon of the second experimental week. This
time frame was similar to several previous studies about impression development
in CMC [Walther, 1993b]. Participants in each of
the four experimental groups were told to rate only one of the four designated
discussants and to e-mail their responses back to the experimenter within
24 hours after receiving the instruments.
3 Results
1. The experimenPreliminary statistical ( and X2) results
of demographic variables indicated that there were no significant differences
among the four experimental groups in terms of participants' characteristics.
In addition, statistical assumptions were checked before statistical analysis
and results indicated that impression scores at Time 1 and Time 2 were
normally distributed and none of the ANOVA assumptions were violated5.
The detailed statistical results of the impression scores at Time 1 and
Time 2 are presented in Tables 1 to 6.
First, Tables 1 and 2 below indicated that there were no interaction
effects between frequency and duration on impression scores at Time 1 (
(1, 82) = 1.35, (
> .05) and at Time 2 (
(1, 82) = 1.82, (
> .05). Therefore, hypothesis 1 was not supported.
[5] The
Levene's test from SPSS 10.0 indicated no severe departure from homogeneity
across the groups. In addition, the boxplot tests from SPSS 10.0 indicated
no severe departure from multivariate normality observed across the groups.

Table 1: Analysis of Variance for Impression Scores at Time
1 (N = 83)

Table 2: Analysis of Variance for Impression Scores at Time
2 (N = 83) 1.
Second, Tables 1 and 2 also indicated that frequency had significant
main effects on impression scores at Time 1 ( (1,
82) = 8.33, (
< .01) and at Time 2 (
(1, 82) = 8.20, (
< .01). Similarly, duration had significant main effects on impression
scores at Time 1 (
(1, 82) = 4.59, (
< .05) and at Time 2 ( (1,
82) = 6.19, (
< .05). Therefore, hypotheses 2 and 3 were supported. 1.
In addition, in order to understand the proportion of variance explained
by each independent variable, Hays' (1988) Omega Squared ((2) was calculated
separately to estimate the strength of association between the independent
variables and the dependent variables at both Time 1 and Time 2. Specifically,
the calculation formulas for a two-factor ANOVA are: (1) factor A: SSa
- (j - 1) * Mserror /Mserror + Sstotal, where j is the number of levels
(groups) for factor a; (2) factor B: SSb - (k - 1) * MSerror/Mserror +
Sstotal, where k is the number of levels for factor b; and (3) factor AB:
Ssinteraction - (j-1) (k-1) * MSerror/Mserror + Sstotal. The values of
Omega Squared for each significant outcome at Time 1 and Time 2 are also
presented in the above two tables, respectively. Tables 1 and 2 indicate
that there was a medium association between frequency and impression scores
at Time 1 (
= .078) and at Time 2 (
= .075). Both associations were statistically significant. In addition,
compared with frequency, the associations between duration and impression
scores were smaller at Time 1 (
= .038) and Time 2 (
= .054).
Third, since there was no significant interaction between frequency
and duration, it is necessary to more clearly understand the specific differences
in impression scores at Time 1 and Time 2 among the four experimental groups.
Thus the Bonferroni multiple comparisons procedure [Sincich
1993] was conducted.
According to Tables 3 and 4, the results of the Bonferroni multiple
comparisons procedure indicated that there were significant differences
between the group of short duration/low frequency and the group of long
duration/high frequency, as well as between the group of short duration/low
frequency and the group of short duration/high frequency at both Time 1
and Time 2. In addition, there were significant differences between the
group of short duration/low frequency and the group of long duration/low
frequency at Time 2.
(I) Group
|
(J) Group
|
Mean Difference (I-J)
|
1
|
2
|
3.23**
|
|
3
|
1.11
|
|
4
|
.63
|
2
|
1
|
-3.23**
|
|
3
|
-2.12
|
|
4
|
-2.60*
|
3
|
1
|
-.11
|
|
2
|
-2.12
|
|
4
|
-.48
|
4
|
1
|
-.63
|
|
2
|
2.608*
|
|
3
|
-.48
|
Note. 1 = Group 1 (long duration/high frequency), 2 = Group 2
(short duration/low frequency), 3 = Group 3 (long duration/low frequency),
and 4 = Group 4 (short duration/high frequency).
< .05. <
.01.
Table 3: Results of Bonferroni Multiple Comparisons among
Impression Scores at Time 1 (N=83)
(J) Group |
(J) Group |
Mean Difference (I-J) |
1 |
2 |
2.86** |
|
3 |
.81 |
|
4 |
.61 |
2 |
1 |
-2.86** |
|
3 |
-2.05* |
|
4 |
-25* |
3 |
1 |
-.81 |
|
2.05 |
* |
|
4 |
-.20 |
4 |
1 |
-.61 |
|
2 |
2.25* |
|
3 |
.20 |
Note. 1 = Group 1 (long duration/high frequency), 2 = Group 2
(short duration/low frequency), 3 = Group 3 (long duration/low frequency),
and 4 = Group 4 (short duration/high frequency).
< .05. <
.01.
Table 4: Results of Bonferroni Multiple Comparisons among
Impression Scores at Time 2 (N=83)
In addition, since this study involved a very imbalanced toward female
group in subject pool, the mean difference in terms of the impression scores
between female and male groups at Time 1 and Time 2 are conducted and presented
in Table 5. The two paired
tests in Table 5 indicated that there was no significant difference in
terms of impression scores between the two gender groups (
> .05) at Time 1 or Time 2 although the male group involved a higher
mean score on two occasions.

Table 5: Means and Standard Deviations for Male and Female
Groups on Impression Scores and t Tests between Males (n = 24) and Females
(n = 59) at Time 1 and Time 2
Fourth, Table 6 below shows the means and standard deviations on impression
scores for the four groups formed by the 2x2 combinations at Time 1 and
Time 2, as well as the
test results of impression scores between Time 1 and Time 2 in all four
groups. The four paired t tests in Table 6 indicated that there was no
significant difference in impression scores between Time 1 and Time 2 in
any of the above four experimental groups (
> .05). Therefore, hypothesis 4 was not supported.

Table 6: Means and Standard Deviations for 2x2 Groups on
Impression Scores and t Tests between Time 1 and Time 2 (N = 83)
4 Discussion
CMC is now a popular mode of communication. Like FtF, CMC involves not
only verbal cues, but also certain nonverbal cues [Liu
and Ginther 1999a, 1999b; Walther
and Tidwell 1995]. Most recent research in the area of CMC has focused
on comparison studies between CMC and FtF environments involving both task-oriented
and social-emotion-oriented models [Lawrence and Mongeau
1996; Walther and Burgoon 1992]. However, the
present study took another quite different methodology, which emphasized
the effects of two nonverbal cues on impression development in CMC environments.
Specifically, this laboratory study explored the effects of frequency and
duration of messaging on impression scores in CMC. Generally speaking,
two hypotheses (hypotheses 2 and 3) in this study were supported, while
two others (hypotheses 1 and 4) were not. As stated previously, there were
no interaction effects between duration and frequency on impression scores
in CMC. However, frequency and duration both had significant main effects
on impression scores in CMC. Therefore, the main effects of frequency and
duration on impression scores in CMC are discussed below.
First, contrary to the expectations, hypothesis 1 was not supported.
That is, there were no interaction effects between duration and frequency
of messaging on impression scores in CMC. This result was different from
the results in previous studies within FtF environments [Koomen
and Sagel 1977] and previous predictions in CMC environments [Hiltz
and Turoff 1978; Rice and Love 1987]. According
to Rice and Love, as well as Hiltz and Turoff [Hilz
and Turoff 1978], duration and frequency of messaging in CMC, similar
to duration and latency of verbal response in FtF, should have significant
interaction effects predicting an individual's participation in CMC group
communication. They all maintained that long duration and high frequency
are related to greater perceived sociability and leadership in group interaction
in CMC environments.
Second, hypothesis 2 was supported. Frequency of messaging had significant
main effects on impression scores in CMC. That is, high frequency resulted
in higher impression scores than low frequency in CMC at both Time 1 and
Time 2. This result was well demonstrated in groups 1 (long duration/high
frequency) and 4 (short duration/high frequency). Both of these groups
involved high frequency and developed higher impression scores than the
other two groups involving low frequency. This result was in agreement
with the results in some previous studies in FtF environments [Willard
and Strodtbeck 1972]. Therefore, it appears that frequency of messaging
is an important factor in CMC and may have similar effects on impression
development in both FtF and CMC environments. So, frequency of messaging
is a good predictor of an individual's participation and impression in
small group interaction in CMC environments. Specifically, CMC users with
high frequency are perceived as being competent and having greater participation,
while those with low frequency are perceived as less competent and having
less participation in CMC.
Third, hypothesis 3 was supported. Duration of messaging had significant
main effects on impression scores in CMC. That is, longer duration resulted
in higher impression scores than shorter duration in CMC at both Time 1
and Time 2. This result was well demonstrated in groups 1 (long duration/high
frequency) and 3 (long duration/low frequency).
Both of these groups involved long duration and developed higher impression
scores than group 2 which involved short duration. This result was in agreement
with the results in some previous studies in FtF environments [Koomen
and Sagel 1977]. Therefore, it appears that duration of messaging is
also an important factor in CMC and may have similar effects on impression
development in both FtF and CMC environments. So, duration of messaging
is also a good predictor of an individual's participation and impression
in small group interaction within CMC environments. Specifically, CMC users
with shorter duration are perceived as less competent and lacking confidence,
while persons with longer duration are perceived as competent and confident.
In all, hypotheses 2 and 4 partially support the predictions of Hiltz
and Turoff [Hiltz and Turoff 1978] and Rice and
Love [Rice and Love 1987] who maintained that frequency
and duration of messaging have similar effects in both FtF and CMC environments
and that both are important predictors of an CMC user's participation in
group communication. In addition, these results partially support Walther's
Social Information Processing Model [Walther 1992]
. According to Walther, CMC users can process all relational cues available
and social identity using various media to present and solicit their relational
behaviors. Specifically, in relation to this study, CMC users can manipulate
nonverbal cues such as duration and frequency of messaging to more favorably
present their behaviors and to achieve better impressions from their partners.
Fourth, contrary to the expectations, hypothesis 4 was not supported.
That is, impression scores did not significantly increase from the end
of the first week to the end of the second week. This finding seems not
to support Walther's initial hypothesis of gradual impression development
in CMC [Walther 1993a, 1993b].
According to Walther, CMC users can gradually develop impressions of their
own partners over time. However, in this study, visual presentation showed
that the means at Time 2 were all higher than the means at Time 1 in terms
of impression scores in spite of no significant differences between the
two occasions in all four experimental groups.
In all, there may be several reasons why the present study fails to
completely support Rice and Love's [Rice and Love 1987]
and Hiltz and Turoff [Hiltz and Turoff 1978] predictions
of the interaction effects and Walther's (Walther 1993a,
1993b] hypothesis of gradual impression development
over time in CMC. These may include, are not limited to: (1) Experimental
duration: This study only involved a period of two weeks, while most prior
CMC studies used a longer period of time such as five weeks. (2) Participants
as observers: Participants in this study were only allowed to be "lurkers"
rather than participants, while most prior CMC studies allowed participants
to participate actively in the list discussions. (3) Composition of participants:
There was a very imbalanced toward female group in subject pool in this
study, while most prior CMC studies did not involve such a high percentage
of females. (4) Experimental tasks: This study only involved one task,
while most prior CMC studies used more tasks for participants to discuss
with each other. In addition, this study involved an emotional topic in
order to stimulate the participants' interests. (5) Research type: This
study only involved the manipulation of two independent variables in CMC
environments, while most prior CMC studies involved the comparison of FtF
and CMC environments. (6) Characteristics of participants: This study involved
participants mostly having CMC experiences, while most prior CMC studies
used participants with no CMC experience. (7) Other relevant factors: These
may include group sizes and language styles.
Finally, as CMC is becoming more and more widely used, there is an urgent
need to study the affects of interactive CMC in different areas, such as
e-learning, e-commerce, and e-health. Based on the findings in this laboratory
study and Walther's [Walther 1992] social information
processing model in CMC, the following recommendations are proposed for
future research in different interactive CMC environments:
- Investigating the effects of frequency and duration of messaging in
interactive CMC discussion in different areas, such as education, work,
commerce, or health.
- Extending the experimental duration to longer periods so that participants
can have enough time to develop impressions and relations.
- Adding more discussion topics in CMC so that participants will not
get bored in the experiment.
- Increasing participants' duration of messaging in extended periods
of communication.
- Using participants from a variety of settings, including educational,
industrial, organizational, and personal communications.
- Recruiting a more balanced group of males and females in subject pool.
5 Conclusion
Although the findings in this experimental study are exploratory, and
further study is needed in interactive CMC environments with different
samples, the initial results are promising. The results suggest that frequency
and duration of messaging are potentially important variables in CMC group
communication; high frequency and long duration can help CMC users achieve
more developed impression from their CMC partners. This conclusion is in
contrast to the task-oriented model of CMC discussed previously. The authors
agree with this conclusion since it can help explain lots of interactive
CMC phenomena. Therefore, the present study generally supports one of CMC's
two major theoretical models-the social-emotion-oriented model in CMC environments
described previously. In other words, all types of CMC users may control
the frequency and duration of messaging to achieve desired impression development
in various CMC environments. In addition, the results in this study has
laid foundations for future research in many popular types of interactive
CMC environments, including e-learning, e-commerce, and e-health.
References
[Adkins and Brashers 1995] Adkins, M., Brashers,
D. E.: "The Power Of Language In Computer-Mediated Groups"; Management
Communication Quarterly, 8, 3 (1995), 289-322.
[American Psychological Association 1994] American
Psychological Association: "Publication Manual of the American Psychological
Association" (4th ed.). American Psychological Association/Washington,
DC (1994).
[Asch 1946] Asch, S. E.: "Forming Impressions
Of Personality"; Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 1
(1946), 258-290.
[Asteroff 1987] Asteroff, J. F.: "Paralanguage
In Electronic Mail: A Case Study"; Unpublished doctoral Dissertation,
Columbia University (1987).
[Breazeale 1999] Breazeale, S. C.: "A Meeting
Of Minds: The Alumni-L Listserv(R) (Administrators, Computer Mediated Communication,
Professional Community)"; (Doctoral dissertation, University of South
Carolina); Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 4 (1999), AAT 9928286.
[Bradac and Street 1989/90] Bradac, J. J., Street,
R. L. Jr.: "Powerful And Powerless Styles Of Talk: A Theoretical Analysis
Of Language And Impression Formation"; Research on Language and Social
Interaction, 23 (1989/90), 195-242.
[Connolly, Jessup and Valacich 1990] Connolly,
T., Jessup, L. M., Valacich, J. S.: "Effects of Anonymity And Evaluative
Tone On Idea Generation In Computer-Mediated Groups"; Management Science,
36, 6 (1990), 689-703.
[Culnan and Markus 1987] Culnan, M., Markus, M.
L.: "Information Technologies". In F. M. Jablin, L. L. Putnam,
K. H. Roberts, & L. W. Porter, (Eds.), Handbook Of Organizational Communication:
An Interdisciplinary Perspective (pp. 420-444). Newbury Park, CA: Sage
(1987).
[Daft and Lengel 1984] Daft, R., Lengel, R. H.:
"Information Richness: A New Approach To Managerial Behavior And Organization
Design", Research in Organizational Behavior, 6 (1984), 191-233.
[Daft and Lengel 1986] Daft, R., Lengel, R. H.:
"Organizational Information Requirements, Media Richness And Structural
Design", Management Science, 32 (1986), 554-571.
[Halbert 1999] Halbert, C. L.: "The Presentation
Of Self In Computer-Mediated Communication: Managing And Challenging Gender
Identity (Impression Management, Chatrooms)" (Doctoral dissertation,
University of Kentucky); Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 8 (1999),
AAT 9943012.
[Hampson 1990] Hampson, S. E.: "Reconciling
Inconsistent Information: Impressions Of Personality From Combinations
Of Traits"; European Journal of Personality, 4 (1990), 157-172.
[Hays 1988] Hays, W.: "Statistics" (4th
ed.); Holt, Rinehart and Winston (pp. 453.) (1988).
[Hesse, Werner and Altman 1988] Hesse, B. W.,
Werner, C. M., Altman, I.: "Temporal Aspects Of Computer-Mediated
Communication"; Computers in Human Behavior, 4 (1988), 147-165.
[Hiltz, Johnson and Turoff 1986] Hiltz, S. R.,
Johnson, K., Turoff, M.: "Experiments In Group Decision Making: Communication
Process And Outcome In Face-To-Face Versus Computerized Conferences";
Human Communication Research, 13 (1986), 225-252.
[Hiltz and Turoff 1978] Hiltz, S. R., Turoff,
M.: "The Network Nation"; Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley (1978).
[Jacobson 1999] Jacobson, D.: "Impression Formation
In Cyberspace: Online Expectations And Offline Experiences In Text-Based
Virtual Communities"; Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication
[Kiesler, Siegel and McGuire 1984] Kiesler, S.,
Siegel, J., McGuire, T. W.: "Social Psychological Aspects Of Computer-Mediated
Communication"; American Psychologist, 39, 10 (1984), 1123-1134.
[Koomen and Sagel 1977] Koomen, W., Sagel, P. K.:
"The Prediction Of Participation In Two-Person Groups"; Sociometry,
40, 4 (1977), 369-373.
[Krauss and Fussell 1996] Krauss, R. M., Fussell,
S. R.: "Social Psychological Models Of Interpersonal Communication";
In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook
of Basic Principles (pp. 655-701), NY: The Guilford Press (1996).
[Kraut 1978] Kraut, R. E.: "Verbal And Nonverbal
Cues In The Perception Of Lying"; Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 36 (1978), 380-391.
[Krout, Lewis and Swezey 1982] Krout, R. E.,
Lewis, S. H., Swezey, L. W.: "Listener Responsiveness And The Coordination
Of Conversation"; Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43
(1982), 718-731.
[Krout, Lewis and Swezey 1996] Lawrence, J.
A., Mongeau, P.: "The Role Of Image Anticipation Of Future Interaction
In Computer-Mediated Impression Development". Paper presented at the
International Communication Association, Chicago, IL (1996, May).
[Lawrence and Mongeau 1996] Lawrence, J. A.,
Mongeau, P.: "The Role of Image Anticipation of Future Interaction
in Computer-Mediated Impression Development". Paper presented at the
International Communication Association, Chicago, IL. (1996).
[Lea and Spears 1991] Lea, M., Spears, R.:
"Computer-Mediated Communication, Deindividualization And Group Decision-Making",
International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 34 (1991), 283-301.
[Liu and Ginther 1999a] Liu, Y., Ginther, D.:
"A Comparison Of Task-Oriented Model And Social-Emotion-Oriented Model
In Computer-Mediated Communication"; Commerce, Texas (ERIC Document
Reproduction Service Number ED 437 924) (1999a).
[Liu and Ginther 1999b] Liu, Y., Ginther, D.:
"How To Achieve A Better Impression In Computer-Mediated Communication";
Commerce, Texas (ERIC Document Reproduction Service Number ED 437 035)
(1999b).
[MacKinnon 1995] MacKinnon, R. C.: "Searching
For The Leviathan In Usenet"; In S. G. Jones (Ed.), Cybersociety:
Computer-mediated Communication and Community (pp. 112-137). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage (1995).
[O'Keefe and Delia 1982] O'Keefe, B. J., Delia,
J. G.: "Impression Formation And Message Production"; In M. E.
Roloff & C. R. Berger (Eds.), Social Cognition and Communication (pp.
33-72). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage (1982).
[Parks and Floyd 1996] Parks, M. R., Floyd, K.:
"Making Friends In Cyberspace"; Journal of Communication; 46,
1 (1996), 80-97.
[Patterson 1994] Patterson, M. L.: "Strategic
Functions Of Nonverbal Exchange"; In J. A. Daly & J. M. Wiemann
(Eds.), Strategic Interpersonal Communication (pp. 273-293), Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum (1994).
[Reid 1995] Reid, E.: "Virtual Worlds: Culture
And Imagination"; In S. G. Jones (Ed.), Cybersociety: Computer-mediated
Communication and Community (pp. 164-183). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage (1995).
[Rice 1984a] Rice, R. E.: "Mediated Group
Communication". In R. E. Rice and Associates (Eds.), The New Media:
Communication, Research, and Technology (pp. 129-154). Beverly Hills, CA:
Sage (1984).
[Rice 1984b] Rice, R. E.: "Evaluating New
Medium System". In J. Johnstone (Ed.), Evaluating the New Information
Technologies: New Directions for Program Evaluation (No. 23, pp. 53-71).
San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass (1984).
[Rice 1990] Rice, R. E.: "Computer-Mediated
Communication System Network Data: Theoretical Concerns And Empirical Examples";
International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 32 (1990), 627-647.
[Rice and Love 1987] Rice, R. E., Love, G.:
"Electronic Emotion: Socioemotional Content In A Computer-Mediated
Communication Network"; Communication Research, 14 (1987), 85-108.
[Rintel and Pittam 1997] Rintel, E. S., Pittam,
J.: "Strangers In A Strange Land: Interaction Management On Internet
Relay Chat"; Human Communication Research, 23, 4 (1997), 507-534.
[Short, Williams and Christie 1976] Short, J.
S., Williams, E., Christie, B.: "The Social Psychology Of Telecommunications";
London: John Wiley and Sons (1976).
[Sincich 1993] Sincich, T.: "Statistics by
Example" (5th ed.); NY: Macmillan (1993).
[Tangmanee 1999] Tangmanee, C.: "The Use Of
Computer-Mediated Communication Systems By Programmers" (Doctoral
dissertation, Syracuse University); Dissertation Abstracts International,
60, 8 (1999), AAT 9940573.
[Thompsen and Foulger 1996] Thompsen, P. A., Foulger,
D. A.: "Effects Of Pictographs And Quoting On Flaming In Electronic
Mail"; Computers in Human Behavior, 12, 2 (1996), 225-243.
[Turoff 1978] Turoff, M.: "The EIES Experience:
Electronic Information Exchange System"; Bulletin of the American
Society for Information Science, 4, 5 (1978), 9-10.
[Utz 2000] Utz, S.: "Social Information Processing
In Muds: The Development Of Friendships In Virtual Worlds"; Journal
of Online Behavior [Online 99], 5, 1 (1999). Available: http://www.ascusc.org/jcmc/vol5/issue1/jacobson.html
(Accessed on February 21, 2001).
[Vallee, Johansen and Sprangler 1975] Vallee, J.,
Johansen, R., Sprangler, K.: "The Computer Conference: An Altered
State Of Communication?"; The Futurist, 9, 3 (1975), 116-121.
[Walther 1992] Walther, J. B.: "Interpersonal
Effects In Computer-Mediated Interaction: A Relational Perspective";
Communication Research, 19, 1 (1992), 52-90.
[Walther 1993a] Walther, J. B.: "Construction
And Validation Of A Quantitative Measure Of Impression Development";
Southern Communication Journal, 59 (1993a), 27-33.
[Walther 1993b] Walther, J. B.: "Impression
Development In Computer-Mediated Interaction"; Western Journal of
Communication, 57 (1993b), 381-398.
[Walther 1995] Walther, J. B.: "Relational
Aspects Of Computer-Mediated Communication: Experimental Observations Over
Time"; Organizational Science, 6, 2 (1995), 186-203.
[Walther 1996] Walther, J. B.: "Computer-Mediated
Communication: Impersonal, Interpersonal, And Hyperpersonal Interaction";
Communication Research, 23, 1 (1996), 3-43.
[Walther, Anderson and Park 1994] Walther,
J. B., Anderson, J. F., Park, D. W.: "Interpersonal Effects In Computer-Mediated
Interaction: A Meta-Analysis Of Social And Antisocial Communication";
Communication Research, 21, 4 (1994), 460-487.
[Walther and Burgoon 1992] Walther, J. B., Burgoon,
J. K.: "Relational Communication In Computer-Mediated Interaction";
Human Communication Research, 19, 1 (1992), 50-88.
[Walther and Tidwell, 1995] Walther, J. B., Tidwell,
L. C.: "Nonverbal Cues In Computer-Mediated Communication, And The
Effect Of Chronemics On Relational Communication"; Journal of Organizational
Computing, 5, 4 (1995), 355-378.
[Willard and Strodtbeck 1972] Willard, D., Strodtbeck,
F.: "Latency Of Verbal Response And Participation In Small Groups";
Sociometry, 35, 1 (1972), 161-175.
[Wright 1999] Wright, K. B.: "Computer-Mediated
Social Support, Older Adults, And Coping" (Doctoral dissertation,
University of Oklahoma, 1999); Dissertation Abstracts International, 60,
1 (1999), AAT 9918762.
|