![]() |
![]() |
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
![]() |
![]() |
User: anonymous |
![]() |
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
In view of the proposed model for knowledge integration, the following considerations can be made for the use of collaborative visualization tools. Visualization can be employed not only to visualize the content of a conversation, but also to communicate social cues as for example the amount of contributions of each converser [DiMicco, et al. 2004, Sack 2000]. Especially in computer mediated conversations, where conversers lack social information (i.e. body language, intonation), the visualization of social information can facilitate sense-making [Smith and Fiore 2001]. DiMicco, Pandolfo, and Bender [DiMicco, Pandolfo, and Bender 2004] found that in a collocated setting, providing visual cues on the amount of contributions of each converser made much-talkers limit their amount of contributions and equilibrated participation. Visuals that are developed within the course of a conversation help participants to keep in mind the current state of the conversation and can be used a mnemonic device of what has been discussed earlier on and what are open issues in the conversation [Kraut, et al. 2003]. Dynamic visuals serve as artefacts and real time persistent reference points around which conversers can coordinate their contributions, both in terms of time and content. They are constantly reminded of the big picture to which they contribute with their single statements. Several studies have argued for the importance of shared visual spaces in creating common ground among interaction partners [Kraut, et al. 2003, Olson and Olson 2000]. Interactive visuals facilitate the establishment of common ground since they provide communicators with an additional, often metaphoric language [Kraut, et al. 2003] and shared reference points. Since these visuals are dynamic and can be changed throughout the conversation process, the refinement and correction processes (that are important for grounding activities)[Clark 1996] can be achieved not only through verbal communication, but are also supported through the developing visual. Finally, Cecez-Kecmanovic and Dalmaris [Cecez-Kecmanovic and Dalmaris 2000] found that when people can see the representation of a collective understanding or opinion, they can recognize the possible discrepancies with their own understanding. Such differences in opinion and inconsistencies in understanding are more easily detectable if visually depicted. Participants can critically review the various elements and the relationships among them and instead of an uncritical acknowledgement of facts, the visual leads to a certain amount of content conflict. Page 155 Yet, the handling of this conflict tends to be collaborative since the visual implies that all contributions are potentially part of the same image. Finally, the visualization of the idea gives it a physical existence and becomes, to some extent, dissociated from the person. In consequence, criticizing the idea is probably not misunderstood as personal attack. In view of these arguments and findings, we stipulate that the use of a collaborative visualization tool has a moderating effect on the proposed model for knowledge integration on a structural level (but not on a level of the means). In particular, we argue that the four dimensions we have presented for knowledge integration (Figure 1) remain important if conversers are supported by a visualization tool; but they integrate their knowledge mainly by gaining and maintaining a big picture and through the establishment of a common ground and less so through equal participation and conflict. Second, conversers that are supported by a collaborative visualization tool manage to constructively deal with conflict (see: Figure 4, Hm= Moderation Hypothesis). The next section examines these claims empirically. Figure 4: Moderation Effect of the Use of Interactive Visual Tools (Hm1, Hm2, m3, Hm4) 4 A First Empirical Test of the Model and the Visualisation's Moderating Effect4.1 Experimental DesignIn a classroom experiment, we conducted a first preliminary test of the proposed model. In order to test the moderating effect of the use of the content-specific [Weinberger and Mandl 2003], interactive visualization software, we operated with a two group design (tool and non-tool groups). In total, 64 people participated in the experiment, that is 32 respondents for both the tool and non tool condition, and a total of 16 groups. The unit of analysis was set at the individual level. Page 156 4.2 Task and SettingThe task was based on a hidden profile [Stasser 1992] scenario. Prior to the experiment, students received a case study on a small-medium enterprise (SME) and its knowledge management projects. In a one-hour discussion (see: Figure 2), students had to decide which three of the five project proposal they would choose for implementation. Half of the students (in their role as experts) received a case version that provided mainly information on the projects, whereas the other half (the decision maker role) obtained mainly strategic, corporate information. Groups were formed of two `experts' and two `decision makers'. We have used the let's focus Positioner, which is part of the software package let's focus (see Figure 2 and 3). The application is intended to support groups to share information, analyze complex issues and to structure various types of information. The tool provides a large library of interactive diagrams and metaphors and includes functionalities of clustering, annotation, replay, levelling, and overlaying, all of them using simple drag and drop interaction which allow users to visualize their thinking and communication processes in a seamless manner [Eppler 2005]. 4.3 Method of AnalysisThe model we have presented for knowledge integration is an indirect reflective, second order model with multiple mediating constructs [Edwards and Bagozzi 2000]. Information on the operationalization of the variables can be found in the Appendix and, in more detail, in [Mengis 2006]. One remark on the constructive conflict variable seems necessary: Since we wanted to avoid building a third order model (with knowledge integration that is reflected, among others, by constructive conflict that, on its part, is reflected by content and relationship conflict), we decided not to include constructive conflict as a latent construct, but directly introduce content conflict and relationship conflict. For reasons of feasibility, the correlation between the content and relationship conflict (which represents the third precondition for a constructive handling of conflict) has been calculated with traditional correlation analysis and not within AMOS. The hypotheses we have put forward are of a structural nature and cannot be examined through a mean comparison. In view of this situation, but considering our small sample size (64 respondents), we have done a traditional confirmatory factor analysis for the first order latent constructs, and we have then introduced these constructs as observed variables in the AMOS program for structural equation analysis[MacCallum, et al. 1996]. Then, we have conducted a group comparison of the whole model. Even if approaching the analysis in this way, the problem of minimal sample size [Gefen 2000, Jackson 2003] is nevertheless not fully resolved so that this analysis can only be understood as a first inconclusive analysis that helps us to refine the model and our hypotheses for then conducting a study that allows for an analysis with more statistical power. 4.4 Results and DiscussionResults of the confirmatory factor analysis as well as of the descriptive statistics of the first order latent variables can be seen in the Appendix [for detailed results, see Appendix and: Mengis 2006]. For reasons of space limitations, we directly present the results of the structural analysis. Page 157 First, we can confirm the here proposed model for knowledge integration. We have a chi-square of 16.176 and a degree of freedom of 16 (which results even a slight overfit of the model). Considering the small sample size we have, most important are the information theoretical measures, for which we have satisfactory results. AIC (68.18) is lower for the default model than for the saturated model. With regard to the descriptive measures, the GFI (0.92) is higher than 0.9, but not the AGFI (0.80). Pclose is 0.60 and passes the usually required threshold of 0.5, as does the rmsea of 0.01, which needs to be below 0.05. In view of these satisfactory values for the various model of fit measures, we can be rather confident regarding the validity of our model for knowledge integration, yet fully acknowledging the huge limitations in power due to the very limited sample size. Second, we have found that the visualization software has a significant moderating effect on our model for knowledge integration (with a p of 0.010). We have various structural differences, i.e. significant differences in terms of loadings and explained variances. As we have claimed in section 3, we can confirm that, in the tool condition, the common ground (CG) and the big picture (BP) constructs have more weight for the integration of knowledge than in the non-tool condition (Figures 5 and 6) (Hm1 and Hm2 supported).
On the other hand, the conflict constructs are more important in the non-tool condition and conflict is — following our definition (1. moderate content conflict (CC), 2. low relationship conflict (RC), 3. low correlation between CC and RC) - handled in a less constructive manner. In fact, in the unsupported condition, content conflict loads negatively on knowledge integration and explains 47% of its variance. On the other hand, for the groups working with the tool, it is of no importance at all. Relationship conflict is detrimental in both situations, but explains slightly less of the variance of knowledge integration in the tool situation. Finally, for groups working without the visualization tool support, content conflict strongly correlates with relationship conflict, which is not at all true for the non-tool condition (0.50** for non-tool vs. 0.05 for tool). Page 158 All these three findings, give us support that the tool helps conversers to deal constructively with conflict (Hm3 supported). Finally, while in the non-tool situation, conversers rely more on equal participation for the integration of their knowledge; in the tool-condition, `equal participation' is less important (see Figures 4 and 5) (Hm4 supported). Interpreting these findings, we can say that conversers who interact without an interactive visual support, struggle more to integrate their knowledge: They lack common ground and the big picture in the conversation and therefore give more importance to equal participation and conflict. In addition, we have seen that, in the unsupported condition, they do not manage to deal constructively with conflict. Supporting conversations with an interactive, real-time visualization tools helps conversers to integrate their knowledge in that they collaboratively create and maintain the big picture and establish a common ground among them ? without taking content criticism personally. 5 ConclusionIn this paper, we have taken a conversation perspective on the topic of team knowledge integration. Hence, we have proposed a reflective model of knowledge integration that highlights key communication success factors. We have shown that conversers (in particular experts and decision makers) who 1. participates equally at the process of conversation, and who 2. manages to gain an maintain the big picture, and 3. who establishes a sufficient common ground among them, and finally 4. who deals constructively with conflict, can be successful in integrating knowledge in their team decision making. On this basis, we have argued that collaborative visualization tools can positively affect the relationship between these four factors and knowledge integration. Through experimental evidence we have shown that conversers using the tool rely more on the creation of common ground and the big picture when integrating knowledge. Without the visual aid, conversers tend to give more importance to equal participation and have difficulties in dealing with conflict in a constructive way. Future research should examine whether this moderating effect can be replicated in different settings and for different decision making tasks. Additionally, situated or longitudinal studies could be employed to analyse the long term structuring effects of the enactment of such visualization tools in teams. References[Alavi and Tiwana 2002] Alavi, M. and Tiwana, D. E.: "Knowledge Integration in Virtual Teams: The Potential Role of Kms "; Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology", 53, 12 (2002), 1029-1037. [Argyris 1996] Argyris, C.: "Knowledge for Action. A Guide to Overcoming Barriers to Organizational Change. " San Francisco, Jossey-Bass Publishers, (1996). [Argyris and Schon 1978] Argyris, C. and Schön, D. A.: "Organization Learning Ii: Theory, Method and Practice". Reading, MA, Addison-Wesley, (1978). Page 159 [Bregman and Haythornthwaite 2001] Bregman, A. and Haythornthwaite, C., "Radicals of Presentation in Persistent Conversation"; Proc. In Proceedings of the 34th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences, Hawaii (2001). [Cecez-Kecmanovic and Dalmaris 2000] Cecez-Kecmanovic, D. and Dalmaris, P., "Knowledge Mapping as Sensemaking in Organizations"; Proc. Australian Conference on Information Systems (ACIS), Brisbane (2000). [Clark 1996] Clark, H. H.: "Using Language". Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, (1996). [Conklin 2006] Conklin, J.: "Dialogue Mapping. Building Shared Understanding of Wicked Problems". Chichester, John Wiley & Sons, (2006). [De Dreu and Weingart 2003] De Dreu, C. K. W. and Weingart, L. R.: "Task Versus Relationship Conflict, Team Performance, and Team Member Satisfaction: A Meta-Analysis"; Journal of Applied Psychology", 88, 4 (2003), 741-749. [DiMicco, Pandolfo, and Bender 2004] DiMicco, J. M., Pandolfo, A., and Bender, W., "Influencing Group Participation with a Shared Display"; Proc. ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW 2004), Chicago (2004). [Dixon 1997] Dixon, N. M.: "The Hallways of Learning"; Organizational Dynamics", 25, (1997), 23-34. [Edwards and Bagozzi 2000]Edwards, J. R. and Bagozzi, R. P.: "On the Nature and Direction of Relationships between Constructs and Measures"; Psychological Methods", 5, 2 (2000), 155-174. [Eisenhardt et al. 2000] Eisenhardt, K. M., Kahwajy, J. L., and Bourgeois III, L. J.: "How Management Teams Can Have a Good Fight. The Absence of Conflict Is Not Harmony, It's Apathy"; Harvard Business Review", 75, (2000), 77-85. [Eisenhardt and Santos 2000] Eisenhardt, K. M. and Santos, F. M., "Knowledge-Based View: A New Theory of Strategy?" in Handbook of Strategy and Management, A. Pettigrew, H. Thomas, and R. Whittington, Eds. London: Sage, (2000), 139-164. [Eisenhardt and Zbaracki 1992] Eisenhardt, K. M. and Zbaracki, M. J.: "Strategic Decision Making"; Strategic Management Journal", 13, (1992), 17-37. [Ellinor and Gerard 1998] Ellinor, L. and Gerard, G.: "Dialogue: Rediscover the Transforming Power of Conversation". New York, Wiley, (1998). [Eppler 2005] Eppler, M. J., "Let's Focus: A Visual Knowledge Communication Suite Enabling Knowledge Dialogues"; Proc. Fifth International Conference on Knowledge Management Iknow, (2005). [Gefen 2000] Gefen: "Structural Equation Modeling and Regression: Guidelines for Research Practice"; Communications of the Association for Information Systems", 4, 7 (2000), 1-70. [Grant 1996] Grant, R. M.: "Prospering in Dynamically-Competitive Environments: Organizational Capability as Knowledge Integration"; Organization Science", 7, 4 (1996), 375-387. [Harkins 1999] Harkins, P. J.: "Powerful Conversations. How High-Impact Leaders Communicate". New York, Mc-Graw-Hill, (1999). [Jackson 2003] Jackson, D. J.: "Revisiting Sample Size and Number of Parameter Estimates: Some Support for the N:Q Hypothesis"; Structural Equation Modeling", 10, 1 (2003), 128-141. [Krauss and Fussell 1991] Krauss, R. M. and Fussell, S. R.: "Perspective-Taking in Communication: Representations of Others' Knowledge in Reference"; Social Cognition", 9, (1991), 2-24. [Kraut, Fussell, and Siegel 2003] Kraut, R. E., Fussell, S. R., and Siegel, J.: "Visual Information as a Conversational Resource in Collaborative Physical Talks"; Human-Computer Interaction", 18, 1 (2003), 13-49. [Levin and Lieberman 2004] Levin, G. and Lieberman, Z., "In-Situ Speech Visualization in Real-Time Interactive Installation and Performance"; Proc. Third International Symposium on Non Photorealistic Animation and Rendering (NPAR 2004), Annecy (France) (2004). Page 160 [MacCallum, et al. 1996] MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., and Sugawara, H. M.: "Power Analysis and Determination of Sample Size for Covariance Structure Modeling"; Psychological Methods", 1, (1996), 130-149. [Mengis 2006] Mengis, J.: "Knowledge in Face-to-Face Communication and the Moderating Effect of Interactive Visualization Tools"; Working Paper ICA ", 1/2006, (2006). [Mengis and Eppler 2005] Mengis, J. and Eppler, M. J., "Inside the Black Box of Knowledge Integration. Managing Knowledge Intensive Conversations "; Proc. European Academy of Management Conference (EURAM) 2005, Munich (2005). [Nonaka and Takeuchi 1995] Nonaka, I. and Takeuchi, H.: "The Knowledge-Creating Company. How Japanese Companies Create the Dynamics of Innovation". New York, Oxford University Press, (1995). [Olson and Olson 2000] Olson, G. M. and Olson, J. S.: "Distance Matters"; Human-Computer Interaction", 15, (2000), 139-178. [Rhodes 1991] Rhodes, J.: "Conceptual Toolmaking. Expert Systems of the Mind". Cambridge, MA, Basil Blackwell, (1991). [Sack 2000] Sack, W., "Discourse Diagrams: Interface Design for Very Large-Scale Conversations. " Proc. 33rd Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences, IEEE Computer Society Press, Los Alamitos CA (2000). [Senge 1990] Senge, P. M.: "The Fifth Discipline. The Art and Practice of the Learning Organization". New York, Currency Doubleday, (1990). [Smith 2001] Smith, M. A. and Fiore, A. T.: "Visualization Components for Persistent Conversations"; Chi ", 3, 1 (2001), 136-143. [Stasser 1992] Stasser, G.: "Information Salience and the Discovery of Hidden Profiles by Decision-Making Groups: A 'Thought Experiment'"; Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes", 52, (1992), 156-161. [Sull, Ghoshal, and Monteiro 2005] Sull, D. N., Ghoshal, S., and Monteiro, F.: "The Hub of the World "; Business Strategy Review", 16, 1 (2005). [Tannen 1999] Tannen, D.: "The Argument Culture: Stopping America's War of Words. " New York, Ballantine, (1999). [Weinberger and Mandl 2003] Weinberger, A. and Mandl, H.: "Computer-Mediated Knowledge Communication"; Studies in Communication Sciences", (2003), 81-105. Page 161 AppendixFactor Loadings, Percentages of Variance Explained, Cronbach Alphas, Mean Values, and Standard Deviations of First Order Latent Variables
Page 162 |
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
![]() |
![]() |